МегаПредмет

ПОЗНАВАТЕЛЬНОЕ

Сила воли ведет к действию, а позитивные действия формируют позитивное отношение


Как определить диапазон голоса - ваш вокал


Игровые автоматы с быстрым выводом


Как цель узнает о ваших желаниях прежде, чем вы начнете действовать. Как компании прогнозируют привычки и манипулируют ими


Целительная привычка


Как самому избавиться от обидчивости


Противоречивые взгляды на качества, присущие мужчинам


Тренинг уверенности в себе


Вкуснейший "Салат из свеклы с чесноком"


Натюрморт и его изобразительные возможности


Применение, как принимать мумие? Мумие для волос, лица, при переломах, при кровотечении и т.д.


Как научиться брать на себя ответственность


Зачем нужны границы в отношениях с детьми?


Световозвращающие элементы на детской одежде


Как победить свой возраст? Восемь уникальных способов, которые помогут достичь долголетия


Как слышать голос Бога


Классификация ожирения по ИМТ (ВОЗ)


Глава 3. Завет мужчины с женщиной


Оси и плоскости тела человека


Оси и плоскости тела человека - Тело человека состоит из определенных топографических частей и участков, в которых расположены органы, мышцы, сосуды, нервы и т.д.


Отёска стен и прирубка косяков Отёска стен и прирубка косяков - Когда на доме не достаёт окон и дверей, красивое высокое крыльцо ещё только в воображении, приходится подниматься с улицы в дом по трапу.


Дифференциальные уравнения второго порядка (модель рынка с прогнозируемыми ценами) Дифференциальные уравнения второго порядка (модель рынка с прогнозируемыми ценами) - В простых моделях рынка спрос и предложение обычно полагают зависящими только от текущей цены на товар.

General Characteristics of Function Words





Function words are words that have little lexical meaning or have ambiguous meaning, but instead serve to express grammatical relationships with other words within a sentence, or specify the attitude or mood of the speaker. Dictionaries define the specific meanings of content words, but can only describe the general usages of function words. By contrast, grammars describe the use of function words in detail, but have little interest in lexical words.

Here follows a list of the type of words included in function words:

articles - the and a - only a, the indefinite article is inflected with an affix n before a noun starting with a vowel in English. In highly inflected languages, the articles take on the case of the declension of the following noun.

pronouns - inflected in English, as he- him, she - her, etc

prepositions - uninflected in English.

conjunctions - uninflected

auxiliary verbs - forming part of the conjugation (pattern of the tenses of main verbs) are always inflected

interjections - sometimes called "filled pauses", are uninflected

particles - convey the attitude of the speaker and are uninflected, as if, then, well, however, thus, etc.

expletives - set up sentences, and other functions, It is, There are, etc.

These words are referred to as “functional” or “grammatical” words because they carry little meaning (have no synonyms) and typically “help” another word. Determiners, for instance, add grammatical information about specificity and definiteness (the dog vs. a dog), but do not essentially alter the meaning. They are also called “closed class words,” since languages do not easily add new words to the set.

Function words are generally unstressed. Therefore function words are prone to contraction--for example, the auxiliary have in I've seen it.

Function words are typically lacking in the speech of young children, certain kinds of aphasia, and telegraphic speech. It is also well known that languages rarely borrow function words from other languages or make up new ones (hence their status as closed class). Most recent innovations in the English vocabulary, such as pizza, angst, fax, e-mail, phat, AIDS, website, browser, screenager, and techno-babble are lexical rather than functional in nature (see, for example, the journal American Speech for lists of new words).

Function words add mainly grammatical information, which means that they are defined above all by their syntactic behavior. Most traditional grammars assumed (and their descendents continue to assume) that the structure of sentences and phrases is determined mainly by lexical words. Function words were regarded as mere additions to lexical phrases. Thus, the sentence The rabbit will see the fox was analysed as a noun phrase the rabbit, followed by a verb phrase will see the fox. The determiner the was thus an addition to the noun phrase, and the auxiliary will was added to the verb phrase.

A shift in this thinking came in the 1980s within the framework of generative grammar. From then on, auxiliaries were attributed with an independent contribution to the sentence structure. However, function words still did not determine the categorial status of a phrase--for example, a phrase such as the rabbit continued to be regarded as a noun phrase containing a determiner.

This view changed radically by the mid-1980s, as function words were increasingly interpreted as the determinants of the categorial status of sentence elements. To use the technical terminology, function words were “projecting to a phrase” or “heading a phrase.” Determiners, for example, came to be regarded as the head of determiner phrases--that is, the rabbit was now interpreted as a determiner phrase the containing the noun phrase rabbit. “Functional projections” were thus assigned a structure similar to “lexical phrases.”

Research in the late 1980s and 1990s revolved around the question of exactly which functional projections a sentence may contain. Each function word expressing a grammatical function was soon regarded as a main structural building block of the sentence. The increasing importance of function words in linguistic theories went hand-in-hand with an increasingly abstract description of sentence structure. This shift provided many empirical and theoretical advantages.

First, sentence structure could now be divided into three functional domains: (1) a lexical domain around the verb, which establishes semantic relations between the main sentence elements; (2) a grammatical domain around the auxiliary, which establishes grammatical relations such as agreement (the auxiliary agrees in number and person with the subject: I am/She is/They are leaving.); (3) a discourse domain around the complementizer that, which links an embedded clause to a main clause (I know that this is true or I wonder whether this is true).

Second, differences between languages could be explained by how the function words, and the domains they define, were used. For instance, the so-called verb-second languages such as German, Middle English, Dutch, and Swedish move the verb to the complementizer domain, whereas languages such as English refrain from doing so. The word order of the equivalent German sentence Yesterday the rabbit saw the fox would thus be Yesterday saw the rabbit the fox. Differences between even unrelated languages were reduced to very basic principles.

Function words and lexical words are not sharply distinct categories but rather form a continuum. Certain classes of words can thus share features with both prototypical lexical words and prototypical function words. The English preposition is a case in point: some prepositions have lexical meaning, such as location (behind) and direction (toward); others have little meaning (of or to). Many are used to introduce sentences (after, for, like) and are therefore similar to prototypical function words, namely complementizers.



In short, function words have little lexical meaning and no stress. In traditional grammars, they do not have their own projection or phrase, whereas in some modern approaches they do. They are very similar (and are historically related) to grammatical markers on lexical words.

 

 





©2015 www.megapredmet.ru Все права принадлежат авторам размещенных материалов.